pharmaceutical

You are currently browsing articles tagged pharmaceutical.

big pharma cartoonA new report due to be published in the May issue of the American Journal of Psychiatry shows that antidepressants aren’t all they’re cracked up to be.

But, faithful readers, you already knew that. Right?

The report is part of the National Institute of Mental Health-funded Sequenced Treatment Alternatives to Relieve Depression (STAR*D) project – the largest study of the treatment of depression conducted in the United States. It showed that findings from clinical studies used to gain FDA approval of antidepressants are not applicable to most patients with depression.

Researchers at the University of Pittsburgh Graduate School of Public Health compared symptoms and outcomes in depressed patients who met phase III study inclusion criteria to those who did not. Phase III studies for antidepressants determine the effectiveness of the drug in comparison to a placebo.

The inclusion criteria for these studies aren’t standardized or subject to any federal guidelines. Typically this means that patients with milder forms of depression, chronic depression, or other psychiatric or medical conditions in addition to short-term depression are excluded from studies.

In other words, the majority of “real world” patients with depression who end up taking antidepressants are excluded from clinical studies. It should be obvious why this is a problem. In a normal, clinical setting many patients with depression do also have other illnesses, such as diabetes, chronic fatigue syndrome or irritable bowel syndrome (IBS). It’s not unusual for them to have anxiety and insomnia, as well. In fact, it wouldn’t be presumptuous to expect that a depressed person might be suffering from a number of conditions that are either contributing to or caused by their illness.

Yet the only people that “qualify” for the clinical trials which determine whether antidepressants get approved by the FDA are those with short-term depression, no history of depression, no other psychiatric conditions such as anxiety, and no physical illnesses like heart disease or diabetes. This is the only subgroup of the general population for which we have any data on the efficacy of antidepressants.

By the same token, this means is that we have almost no clinical data on how antidepressants work for the “real world” patients who are most likely to be taking them. Indeed, after assessing 2,855 patients treated with citalopram (Celexa), the study authors found that fewer than one in four, or 22.2%, of the patients met the usual criteria for inclusion in phase III clinical trials.

According to study lead author, Stephen Wisniewski, Ph.D., professor of epidemiology and co-director of the Epidemiology Data Center, University of Pittsburgh Graduate School of Public Health, “This raises major concerns about whether results from traditional phase III studies can be generalized to most people with depression, who also often suffer from anxiety, substance abuse and other medical and psychiatric problems.”

When Wisniewski and his colleagues looked at the efficacy of antidepressants in those who did not meet phase III inclusion criteria – meaning the majority of people who take the drugs in real life – they found that their outcomes were much worse than those who did qualify for the trials. The depression remission rate in the patients who met the criteria was 34.4 percent, compared to only 24.7 percent in the ineligible group.

So, here’s the bottom line: antidepressants are nowhere near as effective as research suggests.

And that is really bad news for the drug companies, because research already suggests that antidepressants aren’t very effective at all. In fact, as I explained in a previous article, antidepressants are no more effective than placebo for most people. If antidepressants are no more effective than placebo in the patients that do meet phase III criteria, and we know that antidepressants are less effective for patients who don’t meet phase III criteria (the vast majority of “real world” depression patients), then couldn’t we assume that antidepressants are less effective than placebo for most patients?

Yes, we could.

For more information on this topic, check out this index of my articles (as well as selected off-site resources) on depression and antidepressants.

bandaidDrugs comprise the major treatment modality of scientific medicine. A recent article in the New York Times revealed that over half of Americans regularly take prescription drugs for chronic health problems. Sadly, many people don’t realize that the drugs they’re taking could be making their condition worse.

Most drugs don’t cure illness. They just suppress symptoms. Unfortunately, drugs also suppress functions. Though drugs provide symptom relief in the short term, over time they may worsen the underlying condition because they interfere with our body’s self-healing mechanisms. For example, many people take ibuprofen or other non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs (NSAIDs) to cope with arthritis and inflammatory conditions. While NSAIDs are effective in reducing pain and inflammation in the short-term, they are also known to reduce blood flow to cartilage. Since blood carries all of the nutrients and immune substance necessary for tissue repair, NSAIDs can actually worsen the original problem when taken chronically.

The second problem is that, by definition, drugs correct a specific imbalance by causing at least one other and often several other imbalances. When a drug is introduced into the body to address a malfunction in one biochemical pathway, that drug inevitably interacts with many other pathways.

The mapping of these pathways in recent genetic research underscores the danger of pharmaceutical drugs. The diagram below shows the interactions among a small set of cellular proteins found in a fruit fly. Proteins encased in ovals are grouped according to specific pathway functions. Connecting lines indicate protein-protein interactions. Protein interconnections among the different pathways reveal how interfering with one protein may produce profound “side effects” upon other related pathways.1

protein map
Complicating the phenomenon of so-called “side effects” is that biological systems are redundant. The same protein molecule may be used in several different systems of the body, but it has a completely different function in each of them.

Histamine is a perfect example of this. Histamine is a chemical that initiates the cell’s stress response. When histamine is present in the bloodstream of the arms and legs, it starts a local inflammatory reaction in those tissues. But if histamine is present in the blood vessels of the brain, it enhances the growth and function of specialized neurons there.

One of the most amazing features of the body’s signaling system is its specificity. When you have a poison oak rash on your arm, histamine is released in that specific area only to activate an inflammatory response to the allergen. Likewise, if you’re under significant stress, histamine is released only in the brain to enhance the function of neurons.

Unfortunately, pharmaceutical drugs have no such specificity. When you take an antihistamine to deal with the itchiness of an allergic rash, the drug is distributed systemically. It affects histamine receptors wherever they are located throughout the whole body. So, while the antihistamine will curb the blood vessels’ inflammatory response and reduce the allergic symptoms of the rash, it will also enter the brain and affect nerve function – which causes drowsiness.

The recent hormone replacement therapy (HRT) debacle is a tragic example of the inherent risks of pharmaceutical drugs.  Estrogen is best known for its function on the female reproductive system.  However, more recent studies have shown that estrogen also plays an important role in the normal function of blood vessels, the heart and the brain.  That ‘s why synthetic estrogen hormones that were prescribed to alleviate menopausal symptoms ended up causing cardiovascular disease and neural dysfunctions such as strokes.

No matter how “targeted” drugs are, they are still relatively crude, blunt instruments when compared to the body’s highly sophisticated immune system. Prescription drugs are are much more like sledgehammers or shotguns than the “magic bullets” they are made out to be.

In the next article, we’ll take a closer look at the consequences of side effects caused by prescription drugs. Until then, I welcome your questions and comments!

  1. Lipton, B. H. (2008). The Biology of Belief: Unleashing the Power of Consciousness, Matter, & Miracles (illustrated edition.). Hay House.

failureThe U.S. spent 16 percent of its Gross Domestic Product (GDP) – a cool $2 trillion – on health care in 2005.1 Considering this enormous expenditure, we should have the best medicine in the world. We should be reversing disease, preventing disease, and doing minimal harm. However, careful and objective review shows the opposite.

The U.S. ranks just 34th in the world in life expectancy and 29th for infant mortality. Of 13 countries in a recent comparison, the United States ranks an average of 12th (second from bottom) for 16 available health indicators.2

40 million people in this country do not have health insurance. The exorbitant cost of health care seems to be tolerated based on the assumption that better health results from more expensive care, despite studies that as many as 20% to 30% of patients receive contraindicated care.3

Even worse, a recent study by Dr. Barbara Starfield published in 2000 in the prestigious Journal of the American Medical Association demonstrated that iatrogenic incidents (events caused by medical intervention) are the 3rd leading cause of death in this country, causing more than 250,000 deaths per year. Only heart disease and cancer kill more people.

Dr. Starfield estimates that, each year, medical errors and adverse effects of the health care system are responsible for:

  • 116 million extra physician visits
  • 77 million extra prescriptions
  • 17 million emergency department visits
  • 8 million hospitalizations
  • 3 million long-term admissions
  • 199,000 additional deaths
  • $77 billion in extra costs

As grim as they are, these statistics are likely to be seriously underestimated as only about 5 to 20% of iatrogenic incidents are even recordedanalyses which have taken these oversights into consideration estimate that medical care is in fact the leading cause of death in the U.S. each year.

Starfield believes that a major contributor to the poor performance of the United States on health indicators is the high degree of income inequality in this country. Countless studies in the medical literature document the adverse effects of low socioeconomic position on health. New research suggests the adverse effects not only of low social position but, especially, low relative social position in industrialized countries.6

Perhaps the words “health care” have given us the illusion that medicine is about health. In fact, western medicine is not a purveyor of healthcare but of disease-care. When the number one killer in a society is the health care system, that system has no excuse except to address its own urgent shortcomings. Unfortunately, until this happens partaking in allopathic medicine itself is one of the highest causes of death as well as one of the most expensive ways to die.

  1. Park, A. America’s Health Check Up. 11/20/2008. Time Magazine Online.
  2. Starfield B. Primary Care: Balancing Health Needs, Services, and Technology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1998.
  3. Schuster M, McGlynn E, Brook R. How good is the quality of health care in the United States? Milbank Q. 1998;76:517-563
  4. Leape LL. Error in medicine. JAMA . 1994 Dec 21;272(23):1851-7.
  5. injuryboard.com. General Accounting Office study sheds light on nursing home abuse. July 17, 2003 . Available at: http://www.injuryboard.com/view.cfm/Article=3005. Accessed December 17, 2003
  6. Wilkinson R. Unhealthy Societies: The Afflictions of Inequality. London, England: Routledge; 1996.

mouth full of pillsIf you read the papers or watch the news you’ve probably heard about the recently published JUPITER study, advertised with bold headlines such as “Cholesterol drug causes risk of heart attack to plummet” and “Cholesterol-fighting drug shows wider benefit”. If you’ve been following this blog (and perhaps even if you haven’t), you are by now aware that such claims cannot be taken at face value.

You might suspect, for example, that the study was sponsored by a drug company and authored by researchers with financial interests tied to those drug companies. You might wonder if these associations could possibly – just possibly – influence not only the results of the study, but how those results are reported. You might also find yourself questioning the objectivity of a study with the title “Justification for the Use of Statins in Prevention: an Intervention Trial Evaluating Rosuvastatin” (JUPITER).

If you’re asking yourself these questions, you are definitely on the right track. The study was indeed sponsored by a drug company, AstraZeneca. And each author of this study received money in the form of grants, consulting fees and honoraria from pharmaceutical companies – in some cases up to twelve different companies, including AstraZeneca, the study sponsor. Take a look at this list detailing the financial interests of the study authors (now required by the New England Journal of Medicine and other prominent publications):

Dr. Ridker reports receiving grant support from AstraZeneca, Novartis, Merck, Abbott, Roche, and Sanofi-Aventis; consulting fees or lecture fees or both from AstraZeneca, Novartis, Merck, Merck–Schering-Plough, Sanofi-Aventis, Isis, Dade Behring, and Vascular Biogenics; and is listed as a coinventor on patents held by Brigham and Women’s Hospital that relate to the use of inflammatory biomarkers in cardiovascular disease, including the use of high-sensitivity C-reactive protein in the evaluation of patients’ risk of cardiovascular disease. These patents have been licensed to Dade Behring and AstraZeneca. Dr. Fonseca reports receiving research grants, lecture fees, and consulting fees from AstraZeneca, Pfizer, Schering-Plough, Sanofi-Aventis, and Merck; and Dr. Genest, lecture fees from AstraZeneca, Schering-Plough, Merck–Schering-Plough, Pfizer, Novartis, and Sanofi-Aventis and consulting fees from AstraZeneca, Merck, Merck Frosst, Schering-Plough, Pfizer, Novartis, Resverlogix, and Sanofi-Aventis. Dr. Gotto reports receiving consulting fees from Dupont, Novartis, Aegerion, Arisaph, Kowa, Merck, Merck–Schering-Plough, Pfizer, Genentech, Martek, and Reliant; serving as an expert witness; and receiving publication royalties. Dr. Kastelein reports receiving grant support from AstraZeneca, Pfizer, Roche, Novartis, Merck, Merck–Schering-Plough, Isis, Genzyme, and Sanofi-Aventis; lecture fees from AstraZeneca, GlaxoSmithKline, Pfizer, Novartis, Merck–Schering-Plough, Roche, Isis, and Boehringer Ingelheim; and consulting fees from AstraZeneca, Abbott, Pfizer, Isis, Genzyme, Roche, Novartis, Merck, Merck–Schering-Plough, and Sanofi-Aventis. Dr. Koenig reports receiving grant support from AstraZeneca, Roche, Anthera, Dade Behring and GlaxoSmithKline; lecture fees from AstraZeneca, Pfizer, Novartis, GlaxoSmithKline, DiaDexus, Roche, and Boehringer Ingelheim; and consulting fees from GlaxoSmithKline, Medlogix, Anthera, and Roche. Dr. Libby reports receiving lecture fees from Pfizer and lecture or consulting fees from AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, GlaxoSmithKline, Merck, Pfizer, Sanofi-Aventis, VIA Pharmaceuticals, Interleukin Genetics, Kowa Research Institute, Novartis, and Merck–Schering-Plough. Dr. Lorenzatti reports receiving grant support, lecture fees, and consulting fees from AstraZeneca, Takeda, and Novartis; Dr. Nordestgaard, lecture fees from AstraZeneca, Sanofi-Aventis, Pfizer, Boehringer Ingelheim, and Merck and consulting fees from AstraZeneca and BG Medicine; Dr. Shepherd, lecture fees from AstraZeneca, Pfizer, and Merck and consulting fees from AstraZeneca, Merck, Roche, GlaxoSmithKline, Pfizer, Nicox, and Oxford Biosciences; and Dr. Glynn, grant support from AstraZeneca and Bristol-Myers Squibb. No other potential conflict of interest relevant to this article was reported.

Now, the fact that these researchers receive money from all of these drug companies doesn’t mean that they are dishonest or that their data are invalid. However, if you think these conflicts of interest do not influence the outcomes of clinical research, then I suggest you read an article I published a few months ago called When It Comes To Drug Claims, Skepticism Is Healthy.

Now that you’ve put on your “Healthy Skeptic” goggles, we can move on and more closely examine the study itself. There are several things you need to be aware of as we discuss it.

First, although the press articles claim that the study looked at statin use in healthy populations, the subjects were people who had normal cholesterol but high CRP levels. CRP, or C-Reactive Protein, is a measure of inflammation in the body. It is now widely accepted even in the mainstream medical community that inflammation is a major risk factor for heart disease. And because inflammation is a sign of an underlying disease process, these patients were not, in fact, “healthy” as claimed.

There is little doubt that statins reduce inflammation, which can help prevent atherosclerosis. It appears that the benefits of statins are mainly due to this characteristic, rather than to their cholesterol-lowering effects. So it’s no surprise that the statins reduced rates of heart disease and mortality in this population that had inflammation going into the study.

I should also mention, however, that the predictive value of CRP for heart disease is highly controversial. Though some studies show a correlation between high CRP levels and heart disease, many others do not. Many physicians feel that CRP is not a useful indicator in clinical practice.

The second thing you need to be aware of is the difference between relative and absolute risk reduction. Relative risk reduction (RRR) measures how much the risk is reduced in the experimental group compared to a control group. Absolute risk reduction (ARR) is just the absolute difference in outcome rates between the control and treatment groups.

To make this more clear, let’s consider an example. Say that 2000 people enter a study for a particular drug and 1000 of them are randomized to placebo. At the end of the study, one person in the drug group died versus two people in the placebo group. The relative risk reduction of the drug group would thus be 50% (0.002 – 0.001/0.002). That sounds really impressive! The headline for this study might read “New drug reduces chance of dying by 50%!”. While technically true, you can see how misleading this can be. Why? Because when most people read that headline, they will interpret it to mean that if they take that drug, their risk of dying will be reduced by 50%, which is not even close to being true.

The absolute risk reduction, on the other hand, is always a much more modest number. Using the same example above, the absolute risk reduction in the drug group would have been a paltry one-tenth of a percent, or 0.1% (0.002 – 0.001). That’s not a very catchy headline, is it? “New drug reduces risk of dying by one-tenth of a percent”. It just doesn’t grab you the same way. But this is actually a more realistic view of what happened in the study and what we could expect to happen in the real world.

In fact, one could just as accurately say that in this hypothetical study, a patient has a 1-in-1000 (0.1%) chance of their life being saved by the drug. Said another way, 1,000 patients would have to be treated with this drug in order to save a single life. This measurement is called the Needed Number to Treat, and is another means for interpreting the results of clinical trials.

With that in mind, let’s examine the data from the JUPITER study. The actual numbers were 198 deaths out of 8901 in the statin group and 247 deaths out of 8901 in the placebo group. The relative risk reduction for total mortality (deaths) in the drug group was 19.8% [(247/8901 - 198/8901) / (247/8901)]. That means that the risk of death for people taking Crestor was 19.8% smaller than those taking placebo.

But what happens when we look at the absolute risk reduction numbers? According to the data, 2.77% (0.02774) of people taking the placebo died after two years versus 2.24% (0.02224) of people taking Crestor. This amounts to a difference of 0.55%, or one-half of one percent.

Here’s a graphical illustration of the difference in mortality between the Crestor and placebo group:

jupiter graph

If you’re having trouble making much of a difference, I don’t blame you!

To make this even more clear, let’s use the Needed Number to Treat method of evaluating these results. According to the study data, 182 people would have to be treated with Crestor for two years in order to save a single life.

Now that may not sound like a large number to you, especially if yours was one of the lives saved. However, when evaluating the viability of any potential treatment three considerations (above and beyond the efficacy of the treatment) must be taken into account: cost, side effects, and alternatives.

Let’s look at cost first. The cost of one patient taking Crestor for one year is approximately $1,300. Therefore, to prevent 49 deaths 8,901 people would have to take Crestor for two years at a cost of $23 million dollars. That is an enormously expensive treatment by any measure.

Second, this particular study did not register significant side effects in the statin group. This is very fishy, though, since nearly every other study on statins to date has shown significant side effects and the approval of Crestor itself was delayed by the FDA due to concern about Crestor side effects.

While all statins are associated with rare instances of rhabdomyolysis, a breakdown of muscle cells, Crestor had shown in studies before its approval that the potentially deadly disease had surfaced in seven people. Crestor’s potential muscle- and liver-damaging side effects become more worrisome and difficult to justify in patients who are essentially healthy.

What’s more, the study only lasted two years. That’s not long enough to adequately establish safety for the drug, especially if people are going to use it “preventatively”, which means they could be taking it for several years and even decades. Statins have caused cancer in every single animal study to date. Since cancer can take up to 25 years to develop after initial exposure to the carcinogen, we simply cannot know at this point that statins won’t also significantly increase the risk of cancer in adults.

Finally, before jumping on the statin bandwagon and recommending that we spend billions of dollars treating healthy people with Crestor, we should consider if there isn’t a less costly and risky way of preventing deaths due to inflammation and heart disease.

Wouldn’t you know it, there sure is!

For the last decade medical research has increasingly demonstrated that heart disease is caused not by high cholesterol levels, but by inflammation and oxidative damage. A full explanation of these mechanisms is beyond the scope of this post, but for more details you can read two previous articles: Cholesterol Doesn’t Cause Heart Disease and How To Increase Your Risk of Heart Disease.

So, if we want to prevent and even treat heart disease, we need to address the causes of inflammation and oxidative damage. Again, there’s not room to go into great detail on this here but in general the primary causes of inflammation and oxidative damage are 1) a diet high in polyunsaturated oil (PUFA) and refined flour and sugar, 2) lack of physical activity, 3) stress and 4) smoking.

We can thus prevent heart disease by avoiding PUFA and refined/processed food, getting adequate exercise, reducing stress and not smoking. These simple dietary and lifestyle changes are likely to produce even better results than a statin, for a fraction of the cost and without any side effects. In fact, the only side effects of this approach are improved physiological and psychological health! For more specific recommendations, read my article Preventing Heart Disease Without Drugs.

Taking a statin to “prevent” inflammation and heart disease is rather like bailing water with a pail to prevent a boat from sinking instead of simply plugging the leak. Unfortunately, our entire health care system is oriented around “bailing water with a pail”, which is to say treating the symptoms of disease, instead of “plugging the leak”, or addressing the causes of disease before it develops. The reason this is the case is because there’s a lot more money to be made from drugs, surgery and other costly interventions than there is from encouraging people to eat well, exercise and reduce stress.

Even if we ignore all of the issues I’ve pointed out above, the best thing we can say about this study is that a small group of unusual patients – those with low LDL-cholesterol AND high C-reactive protein – may slightly decrease their risk for all-cause mortality by taking a drug that costs them almost $1,300 per year and slightly increases their risk for developing diabetes.

That’s the best spin possible given the data from this study. Compare that to the mainstream media headlines, and you’ll have a clear understanding of how financial conflicts of interest are seriously damaging the integrity and value of clinical research.

At least the media wasn’t completely fooled. They did manage to at least include the perspective of sane doctors who questioned the desirability of millions of relatively healthy people taking drugs for the rest of their lives. According to the Wall Street Journal:

Moreover, despite large relative benefits, the actual number of patients helped was small. Those on the drug suffered 142 major cardiovascular events compared with 251 on placebo, a difference of 109. Dr. Hlatky said that raises questions about the cost-effectiveness of CRP screening and the value of putting millions of low-risk patients on medication for the rest of their lives.

From the New York Times:

Some consumer advocates and doctors raised concerns about the expense of putting relatively healthy patients on statins, which would cost the health system billions of dollars.

From Fox News:

About 120 people would have to take Crestor for two years to prevent a single heart attack, stroke or death, said Stanford University cardiologist Dr. Mark Hlatky. He wrote an editorial accompanying the study published online by the New England Journal of Medicine.

“Everybody likes the idea of prevention. We need to slow down and ask how many people are we going to be treating with drugs for the rest of their lives to prevent heart disease, versus a lot of other things we’re not doing” to improve health, Hlatky said.

If you know of someone who is considering a statin after reading about the JUPITER study, please do them a favor and send them a link to this article first. They should hear both sides of the story before making such a significant decision.

dollar signIn a recent post, I discussed the consequences of the massive conflicts of interest that exist between researchers, doctors and the pharmaceutical industry in the U.S. and abroad.

On June 8th the New York Times published an article underscoring these consequences and illuminating the risks that inevitably come with financial ties between researchers and drug companies.

The article revealed that Dr. Joseph Biederman, a world-renowned child psychiatrist at Harvard, accepted at least $1.6 million in consulting fees from drug makers from 2000 to 2007 but did not disclose any of this income to university officials. By failing to report this income, Dr. Biederman and colleagues may have violated both federal and university research rules designed to prevent conflicts of interest.

Dr. Biederman is one of the most influential researchers in child psychiatry. Although many of his studies are small and often financed by pharmaceutical companies, his work has nevertheless directly contributed to a controversial 40-fold increase from 1994 to 2003 in the diagnosis of pediatric bipolar disorder and a concurrent rise in the use of powerful antipsychotic medicines in children.

We know from my previous post that it has been shown that studies funded by pharmaceutical companies are more likely to show positive results for the drug. We also know that the veracity of clinical trials which are the basis of approval of new drugs by the FDA has been called into question in recent studies because of three major flaws: conflicts of interest on the part of investigators (like Biederman); inappropriate involvement of research sponsors (drug companies) in study design and management; and publication bias in disseminating results (if a study has negative results, the drug company doesn’t publish it).

When a researcher like Dr. Biederman is paid millions by a drug company to study it’s product, we must wonder whether we can expect his work to be objective and accurate. But when that researcher repeatedly lies about the money he received, the integrity of his work should be in serious doubt.

In one revealing example, Dr. Biederman reported no income from Johnson & Johnson for 2001 in a disclosure report filed with Harvard University. When asked to check again, he said he received $3,500. But Johnson & Johnson told Congressional investigators that Mr. Biederman was paid $58,169 in 2001.

The consulting arrangements of Dr. Biederman’s entire research group at Harvard were already controversial because of the researcher’s advocacy of unapproved (”off-label”) uses of psychiatric medicines in children. Dr. Biederman and his colleagues have promoted the aggressive diagnosis and treatment of childhood bipolar disorder with antipsychotic drugs – although these drugs have never been approved for such use. In fact, neuroleptic drugs have not been approved for use in children at all.

As a result of Dr. Biederman’s promotion of both the diagnosis and treatment for childhood bipolar disorder, antipsychotic drug use in children has exploded. Roughly half a million children and teenagers were given at least one prescription for an antipsychotic in 2007, including 20,500 under 6 years of age, according to Medco Health Solutions, a pharmacy benefit manager.

The dramatic increase in antipsychotic prescriptions in children has occurred despite the lack of evidence that these medication improve children’s lives over time. On the contrary, it is well known that children are susceptible to the weight gain and metabolic problems caused by the drugs. Children typically gain twice as much weight in the first six months on atypical neuroleptic drugs (risperidone, olanzapine, etc.) as they should through normal growth, adding an average of 2 to 3 inches to their waistline. This is mostly abdominal fat, which also increases their risk of diabetes and heart disease.

There is also some evidence which suggests that these drugs may cause permanent changes to the structure and function of the brain (Breggin 1997). In other words, they cause brain damage.

The research of Dr. Biederman’s group, which has served as the basis for the rise in bipolar diagnoses and antipsychotic use in children, has been widely criticized by other psychiatrists and researchers.

The studies published by Dr. Biederman’s group were so small and “loosely” designed that they were largely inconclusive. In some studies testing antipsychotic drugs, the group defined improvement as a decline of 30 percent or more on a scale called the Young Mania Rating Scale, which is well below the 50 percent change that most researchers use as the standard.

Controlling for bias in these types of studies is particularly important, given that the scale is subjective and depends on reports from physicians, parents and children.

More broadly, psychiatrists have said that revelations of undisclosed payments from drug makers to leading researchers are especially damaging for psychiatry.

“The price we pay for these kinds of revelations is credibility, and we just can’t afford to lose any more of that in this field,” said Dr. E. Fuller Torrey, executive director of the Stanley Medical Research Institute, which finances psychiatric studies. “In the area of child psychiatry in particular, we know much less than we should, and we desperately need research that is not influenced by industry money.”

I couldn’t have said it better myself.

money in pill bottleI’m preparing for an upcoming presentation in September called “The (Hidden) Truth About Antidepressants”, so I will be writing frequently about issues related to the definition, cause and treatment of depression in the weeks to come.

Much of what I write may challenge your current beliefs and contradict what you’ve heard about depression and antidepressants. My hope is that today’s post about the influence of the pharmaceutical industry on doctors, researchers and patients will inspire you to re-examine what you’ve been told so far and approach everything you hear in the future with a “healthy skepticism”.

The truth is that all of our beliefs about depression have been tainted, quite intentionally, by the more than $20 billion spent each year by pharmaceutical companies to promote their drugs (an amount greater than the gross domestic product of all but 70 of the world’s richest nations). In 2000, the pharmaceutical industry had a combined lobbying and campaign contribution budget of $200 million – larger than any other industry (Wayne & Peterson, 2001). The industry has 625 registered lobbyists, more than there are members of congress (Wayne & Peterson, 2001). The industry also underwrites about 70% of clinical drug trials in the United States (DeAngelis et al., 2001).

Consumer Reports has detailed the marketing strategies used by drug companies, including:

  • giving free samples and information to doctors
  • advertising in medical journals
  • using “ask your doctor” media advertisements aimed directly at the consumer (the U.S. and New Zealand are the only two countries that allow this)
  • sponsoring promotional dinner meetings with substantial gifts or even cash provided for attendees
  • paying consultants to speak at scientific meetings where it is possible to circumvent FDA guidelines that require disclosure of side effects
  • funding only those research projects that have a high likelihood of producing favorable results for a particular drug company’s product
  • terminating negative studies before they are ready for publication
  • not publishing studies with negative results
  • offering to pay journalists to cover their products
  • helping to fund patient advocacy and other public interest groups so the consumer group appears to be publicly carrying the banner of a particular drug

How can we possibly rely on information that is so inexorably intertwined with corporate interests? Corporations have very little motivation to share information that could harm sales of their products, as they are required by law to maximize profits for their shareholders. On the contrary, they have much incentive to do everything in their power to suppress such information. Several studies have shown that researchers who produce data that is contrary to the interests of the pharmaceutical industry risk legal, professional, or even personal attack – directly or indirectly financed by the industry. (Bosley, 2002; Healy, 2002; Monbiot, 2002).

As researcher David Antonuccio points out in his excellent article Antidepressants: A Triumph of Marketing over Science?:

“Company-sponsored experts, whether they are researchers or educators, are by definition company employees. They will be retained only if they offer consistently favorable treatment to the company’s products. It could be argued that their efforts on behalf of antidepressants often fit more properly under the rubric of marketing or advertising, not science or education.”

Clinical trials are the basis of approval of new drugs by the FDA, but their reliability is seriously in doubt because of three major flaws: conflicts of interest on the part of investigators; inappropriate involvement of research sponsors in their design and management; and publication bias in disseminating their results. (Quick, 2001)

The situation has become so dire that in September of 2001 the editors of 13 leading medical journals published a joint editorial in which they said:

“Research contracts should give the researchers a substantial say in trial design, access to the raw data, responsibility for data analysis and interpretation, and the right to publish”

Huh? Wouldn’t you expect researchers to have these rights already? In many cases, they don’t.

The editor of the prestigious New England Journal of Medicine argued in a separate editorial that the editors didn’t go far enough in their rebuke:

“The entire system of clinical investigation is driven by profit. We are seeing the corruption of a system of research that used to have high ideals and be clearly in the public interest.”

The conflicts of interest between researchers and drug companies is bad enough. But what’s even more distressing is that many doctors do not even read the research to learn about the drugs they are prescribing. Jerry Avorn, a Harvard Medical School professor and drug researcher is a leading authority on how physicians are educated about new drugs. He acknowledges that most physicians have only minimal knowledge about drug studies. Instead, Dr. Avorn has this to say about where most physicians get their knowledge about drugs:

“Pharmaceutical marketing is about the most important source of knowledge about new drugs for most physicians, and a major form of continuing education as well.”

There are now over 90,000 pharmaceutical reps walking the halls of medical offices around the U.S. Since there are less than 600,000 office-based doctors in the U.S. today, there is approximately one full-time drug rep for every six physicians. The drug reps bring free food for office staff, free samples for distribution to patients, free pens, free textbooks and other free gifts. They are also sometimes authorized to provide free vacations for physicians who would enjoy spending a weekend with other physicians in places like Hawaii or the Caribbean hearing the latest “research” on the effectiveness of a drug. In 2006, the pharmaceutical industry spent $2 billion on these types of events alone.

Does all of this advertising and promotion actually influence doctors? You bet it does. A government report found that in just one year the most heavily advertised drugs had prescription increases of 25% (U.S. General Accounting Office, 2002). There is even a formula that generally applies to drug advertising: each dollar spent on advertising increases sales by $4.

Even more discouraging than the influence of drug companies on doctors is the influence of patients who’ve been subjected to drug company advertising on doctors! A study published in the Journal of the American Board of Family Practitioners reported that 49% of patient requests for drugs or other requests prompted by “direct-to-consumer” advertising were not clinically appropriate. Yet 7 out of 10 times, physicians gave into the requests. (And that is by their own admission; there is likely a percentage of physicians who do not want to admit they write prescriptions or order tests on the basis of patient requests.)

The influence of advertising on doctors and patients is particularly relevant in the case of antidepressants. By a wide margin the largest amount spent on advertising by drug companies was on antidepressant promotion – a whopping $367 million dollars per week!(U.S. General Accounting Office, 2002) In fact, it appears that DTC advertising may be the single most effective way a drug company can increase the number of people who are diagnosed with depression and then will begin taking antidepressants (Donohue, 2004).

I could go on, but I think you get the point. As consumers and patients we simply cannot rely on profit-driven drug companies to give us accurate information about their products. And unfortunately, because of the massive conflicts of interest that exist between researchers, physicians and the pharmaceutical industry – we cannot necessarily rely on our doctors or even scientific studies to show us the way.

Luckily for us, there are still studies being done by independent researchers and those brave enough to risk the ire of the drug companies that we can turn to for honest, unbiased data. Unsurprisingly, these studies often have very different results than those sponsored by the industry. Thanks to the Freedom of Information Act, some researchers have even been able to access the studies done by the drug companies that they never published (obviously the ones that were least favorable to their drugs).

When these independent and unpublished studies are analyzed, a very different picture of depression and the efficacy of antidepressants begins to emerge.

Contrary to popular belief:

  1. There is no evidence that depression is caused by a “chemical imbalance” (which is the rationale behind prescribing antidepressants).
  2. Recent meta-analyses of the research data show that antidepressants have no clinically meaningful advantage over placebo. Therefore, the term “antidepressant” is a misnomer and should be abandoned.
  3. Poor study design may account for the small degree of superiority shown over placebo
  4. Claims that antidepressants are more effective in more severe conditions have little evidence to support them.
  5. Antidepressants have not been shown to affect the long-term outcome of depression or suicide rates.
  6. It is now recognized that SSRIs (the most widely used class of antidepressants) increase the risk of suicidal behavior in children and adolescents, and there is legitimate concern that the same is true for adults.
  7. Given doubt about their benefits and concern about their risks, current recommendations for prescribing antidepressants should be reconsidered.

You might be shocked by some of these statements. Though I was already very skeptical about antidepressants before beginning this research, I myself have been blown away by the complete lack of evidence supporting the theory that depression is a biological disease and the very strong evidence that antidepressants are no more effective than placebo.

I’ll be writing in more detail about several of the points to come in the coming weeks, so please stay tuned!

capsulesA recent article in the New York Times revealed that over half of Americans are taking prescription medication for chronic health problems.

The numbers were gathered last year by Medco Health Solutions Inc., which manages prescription benefits for about one in five Americans.

The data indicates that 51 percent of American children and adults were taking one or more prescription drugs for a chronic condition, up from 47 percent in 2001. The use of drugs to treat health problems was seen in all demographic groups:

  • Almost two-thirds of women 20 and older
  • One in four children and teenagers
  • 52 percent of adult men
  • Three-quarters of people 65 or older

28 percent of women and 22 percent of men over 65 take five or more medicines regularly.

Exactly what medications are people taking? In 2006, the top five drugs by sales were Lipitor, Nexium, Prevacid, Advair Diskus and Singulair. Lipitor lowers cholesterol, Nexium & Prevacid lower stomach acid, and Advair Diskus and Singulair address asthma and allergies respectively.

The drugs on this list reveal much about the weakness of the mainstream medical model. Cholesterol and stomach acid are both normal, protective substances in the body. Cholesterol is no more the cause of heart disease than stomach acid is the cause of GERD or ulcers. But one of the fundamental flaws of western medicine is its tendency to treat the symptom or effect rather than the cause. Unfortunately for patients, doing so can actually make things worse, not better.

Cholesterol plays many essential roles in the body, and and lowering it arbitrarily not only doesn’t prevent heart disease, but can actually increase the risk of dying from a heart attack in elderly people. Likewise, stomach acid is crucial in protecting us from the pathogens we might otherwise ingest with food. Stomach acid is also required for protein digestion. It is well-established in the scientific literature that the primary cause of ulcers is a bacterium called h. pylori – not stomach acid. And there is also evidence suggesting that GERD (gastro-esophageal reflux disease) is caused by low – not high – stomach acid.

But I digress.

The scariest part of this study is the surge in children’s use of medicines to treat weight-related problems and other illnesses previously considered adult problems. Medco estimates about 1.2 million American children now are taking pills for Type 2 diabetes, sleeping troubles and gastrointestinal problems such as heartburn.

The majority of these conditions – diabetes, sleeping troubles and gastrointestinal issues – can be treated by simple diet and lifestyle changes. These changes have none of the adverse effects and risks of drugs, and their benefits extend far beyond the potential therapeutic action of the medications.

Medication has improved and even saved the lives of many in this country and around the world. Yet there’s a difference between drugs that are “medically necessary” and drugs that are prescribed in lieu of other less harmful and risky – but more labor intensive – interventions such as diet and exercise.

But as Dr. Robert Epstein, cheif medical officer at Franklin Lakes, N.J.-based Medco said, ‘We’ve become a couch potato culture (and) it’s a lot easier to pop a pill” than to exercise regularly or diet.

I couldn’t have said it better myself.

One reason for the increase in medication use is the pharmaceutical industry’s “relentless advertising”. Since that is unlikely to change anytime soon, experts say the proportion of Americans on chronic medications can only multiply.

“Unless we do things to change the way we’re managing health in this country … things will get worse instead of getting better,” predicted Daniel Jones, a heart specialist and dean of the University of Mississippi’s medical school.

Luckily, we don’t have to wait around for that to happen. As individuals we can take responsibility for our own health care using diet, exercise and lifestyle changes. We can choose to use “alternative” modalities such as acupuncture and homeopathy to keep us healthy. And we can take action to reduce stress and promote emotional and psychological well-being.

Recommended Links

  • Americans Taking Prescription Drugs in Greater Numbers

Bad Behavior has blocked 1411 access attempts in the last 7 days.